‘Natures answer to over-conformity’: deconstructing
Pathological Demand Avoidance.
By Damian Milton
Abstract
Throughout
its history autism has been primarily defined in terms of a pathologised
deviancy from normative cognitive functionality, despite protestations to the
contrary from autistic writers (Sinclair, 1993, Arnold, 2010, Milton,
2011). More recently however, we have
witnessed the wider acceptance of a construction concerning a perceived
pervasive developmental disorder known as Pathological Demand Avoidance
syndrome (PDA). This conceptualisation
was first formulated by Elizabeth Newson in the 1980’s (PDA Contact Centre,
2012), yet more recently has been recognised by the National Autistic Society
as a variant of an autistic spectrum disorder (ASD). This paper deconstructs the psychologisation
of autistic agency inherent in the theory supporting PDA, through a personal
reflection of an autistic activist and academic who according to such a
perspective may well have met the criteria for PDA when a child. This paper
concludes by arguing that the label of PDA represents the medicalising and
pathologising of behaviours that from an outsider perspective seem to be
differentiated from what is deemed capable by autistic people, but could be
seen as the behaviours of an autistic person who has gained a modicum of
normative social skills and is simply asserting their agency. By pathologising such behaviour, one could
unduly be blunting attempts at autistic self-advocacy.
Me
A withered boy who was so afraid, hiding from society in the
shade,
His solitary cries no-one did hear, his confused mind full of
fear.
His tortured soul locked inside, with his faded dreams that had
died.
(Milton, 1989).
My own journey through the world of psychiatry and psychology
As a
young child I remember various GPs and teaching professionals being concerned
with the question: “What is wrong with that boy?” Early on though, my oddball quirks were put
down to shyness and eccentricity, and I was diagnosed with Asthma and
Hayfever. My first encounter with
psychiatric professionals came following a road traffic accident and a court
case my mother had started to claim damages through, not only for herself as
she had suffered significant permanent physical injuries, but on behalf of the
mental and emotional damage sustained by me and my brother, with particular
attention paid to me as I seemed to have been particularly adversely affected
(and I had been!). I was eleven years
old at the time. During this process I
was assessed by a variety of psychiatrists and psychologists, each one having a
different theory as to: ‘what is wrong with that boy?’ These explanations ran from ‘psychologically
disturbed’, to personality disorder and catatonic schizophrenia, with some
suggesting that I had not been affected by the trauma of the accident, but had
an underlying condition. Following
unsuccessful encounters with various counselling services I decided to avoid
such professionals at all costs, with a growing fear that the ‘men in white
coats would come to get me’. Indeed, one
could say that throughout my childhood I perfected ways of ‘coping’ with the
imposed will of others, usually through a form of passive defiance (although
not always quite so passive in my teens).
Several later after some years studying sociology and philosophy in an
attempt to reason with the ‘insanity’ of the social world that I found myself
in, I became a father. At the age of two
my son was diagnosed with autism and severe learning difficulties. By researching this diagnosis I realised that
I myself was on the autism spectrum, and was later diagnosed at the age of
thirty-six with Asperger syndrome. Since
then I went on to claim a distinction in Psychology (conversion diploma) from
the Open University and set about deconstructing the social construction of
autism. I am now studying for my
doctorate at the University
of Birmingham researching
the tensions between various stakeholders in the narratives regarding the
education of autistic people.
Pathological Demand Avoidance (PDA)
Throughout
its history autism has been primarily defined in terms of a pathologised
deviancy from normative cognitive functionality, despite protestations to the
contrary from autistic writers (Sinclair, 1993, Arnold, 2010, Milton,
2011).
“...right from the start, from the time someone came up with
the word ‘autism’, the condition has been judged from the outside, by its appearances,
and not from the inside according to how it is experienced.” (Williams, 1996:
14).
More
recently however, we have witnessed the construction of a categorisation
concerning a perceived pervasive developmental disorder known as Pathological Demand
Avoidance syndrome (PDA). This
conceptualisation was first formulated by Elizabeth Newson in the 1980’s (PDA
Contact Centre, 2012), yet more recently has been recognised by the National
Autistic Society as a variant of an autistic spectrum disorder (ASD). Many of the children being diagnosed
initially by the Elizabeth Newson clinic were said to display traits and
characteristics of autism, yet did not show a typical presentation of ‘classic’
autism or Asperger Syndrome, and so were often labelled with ‘Atypical autism’
or PDD-NOS (pervasive developmental disorder – not otherwise specified). These labels were felt to be unhelpful to the
families of these children, thus spurring Newson and her followers to attempt
to identify PDA as a separate syndrome.
Children
characterised with PDA are believed to ‘resist the ordinary demands of life’ to
a degree deemed pathological by clinicians (Newson 2003). Children seen as having PDA are often
described as ‘Jekyll and Hyde’ personalities, involving sharp mood swings and deemed
to display severe ‘challenging behaviour’.
As with the psychological construction of autism more generally, PDA has
increasingly been identified as being produced by a combination of genetic and
environmental factors which alter early brain development. Children characterised as having PDA are said
to have an anxiety-led need to control their environments, possess superficial
social skills, and often engage in manipulative and domineering behaviour. The criteria for PDA as devised by Newson
(2003) includes:
1. Passivity
in early childhood, resisting demands and missing developmental milestones.
2. Continuing
to resist demands, distraction techniques, resorting to meltdowns (panic
attacks) if demands are enforced.
3. Surface
sociability, but apparent lack of sense of social identity, pride or shame.
4. Comfortable
in role play and pretending.
5. Language
delay, seemingly the result of passivity.
6. Obsessive
behaviour.
7. Neurological
signs – such as awkwardness, similar to autism spectrum disorders.
If
one had assessed me as a child against these criteria (or for that matter as I
generally present today), especially within particular contexts, I would have
met them all except perhaps that of being comfortable in role play or
pretending. This difference would not be
enough to separate those diagnosed with Asperger syndrome however from those
deemed to display traits of PDA when one considers successful performers or actors
on the spectrum such as Paddy Constantine.
PDA
is not currently recognised by either the DSM-IV or the ICD-10 nor is it under
consideration for inclusion in the proposals for the new DSM-V. Resistance to
the inclusion of PDA into the DSM being predicated on it being a false
identification of a form of attachment disorder rather than having a
neurological basis (PDA Contact Centre, 2012).
Despite these conceptual issues regarding the causes of the behavioural
manifestations of PDA, the label has in recent times caught the attention of many
psychologists and practitioners working in the field of autism studies.
The
drive behind this label to become accepted in the psychology lexicon has
primarily come from Newson (2003) herself and her successor as a clinician at
the Elizabeth Newson centre: Phil Christie (2007). For Newson (2003) the main difference between
those identified as having PDA rather than autism or Asperger’s is that
children displayed ‘superficial social skills’ and are ‘socially
manipulative’. The children often could also
maintain eye contact. Other factors associated
with PDA included a ‘lack of self-identity’, with an understanding of how
others should behave, but being unable to include themselves in these
expectations, often imitative of ‘inappropriate behaviour’ (with a
recommendation to provide a ‘normal’ peer group to model behaviour on), and an
obsessive need to dominate social interactions and for others to follow their
terms, often coming across as overbearing.
According to the PDA Contact Centre (2012) there are two main types of
PDA: ‘actively passive’ and ‘actively disruptive’, yet with children moving
between these reactions. These
behaviours are seen as not involving choice or agency, but as a manifestation
of not being able to cope with the ‘stress of everyday demands’ due to
‘cognitive deficit and pathology’.
In
contrast to the normative functionalist models of mind and behaviour that are
demonstrated in the vast majority of literature regarding autism and related
pervasive developmental disorders, autistic self-advocates often remark on how
autism can be better understood as a cognitive difference or diversity, and
that the breakdown in communication and empathy impacts of both parties
(Sinclair, 1993, Milton, 2012a). Taking
an interactionist or post-structural approach to social relations, it is quite
bizarre to speak of someone being able to possess definable social ‘skills’
which are not dependent on a multitude of social influences, simply not
attended to by the individualising approach of cognitive functionalism. Thus, in this conceptualisation, what can be
deemed appropriate, challenging, overbearing and so on are negotiated
positionalities. The labelling of the
expressions of autistic agency as pathological is in itself disempowering.
According
to research quoted By the PDA Contact Centre (2012) there are statistically
significant differences between PDA and autism/asperger syndrome, in that PDA
children are less likely to:
-
Have caused anxiety to parents before 18
months of age
-
Show stereotypical motor mannerisms
-
Show (or have shown) echolalia or pronoun
reversal
-
Show speech anomalies in terms of
pragmatics
-
Show (or have shown) tiptoe walking
-
Show compulsive adherence to routines
This
list includes a contradiction however, in that children who are meant to show
‘soft’ neurological signs of awkwardness or clumsiness are also statistically
significantly less likely to walk on tiptoes.
As someone who often paces or walks on tiptoes in order to gain feedback
of where they are in their environment, this seems to be highly unlikely to be
such a significant difference between those potentially characterised as having
PDA or Asperger syndrome. Along with
this list, there is also one regarding attributes found to be statistically
significant in terms of regularity within the group of children thus
characterised:
-
Resist demands obsessively.
-
Be socially manipulative.
-
Show normal eye contact
-
Show excessive lability of mood and
impulsivity.
-
Show social mimicry (includes gestures and
personal style).
-
Show role play (more extended and complete
than mimicry).
-
Show other types of symbolic play.
-
Be female (50%).
If one were to apply these criteria to me,
it would be hard to characterise my actions as ‘manipulative’, and I am
certainly not good at, or motivated by, social mimicry or role play. Newson (2003) and Christie (2007) would
suggest that these differences are definably different to the traits found in
autistic people, yet as said earlier, there are successful performers and
actors that are on the spectrum who do display these skills. When considering such a distinction, Christie
(2007) suggests that it is an important sub-type to distinguish, due to the
idea that children with PDA do not respond well to traditional behavioural
techniques deemed successful with those on the autism spectrum. The obvious weakness with this argument is
that such techniques are not ‘successful’ according to many autistic people
either (Dawson, 2004, Milton, 2012b).
It is
also suggested that children displaying PDA are more likely to become obsessed
with particular individuals or relationships and as utilising bizarre content
in language use, conceived as to be more common than in autism, due to an
interest in fantasy. Similar attributes
however have been made regarding women on the autism spectrum (Simone, 2010) and
it is interesting to note that PDA is considered to be affecting an equal
number of males and females. Could it
just be the case that PDA is an unnecessary extra arbitrary line in the sand?
‘Extremes of any combination come to be seen as
'psychiatric deviance'. In the argument presented here, where disorder begins
is entirely down to social convention, and where one decides to draw the line
across the spectrum.’ (Milton, 1999 - spectrum referring to the 'human spectrum
of dispositional diversity').
The major reason for the growing interest in
PDA, has according to Christie (2007) been in the sense of recognition
expressed by both parents and professionals of the behavioural profile as
described by Newson (2003) and how different it is conceived to be from
conventional understandings of ASD. It
is argued here that such statements are based on flawed misguided theories
regarding what autism is (Milton, 2012c), and thus the supposed differences
between these categories begin to evaporate under closer inspection. Wing and Gould (2002) contend that PDA is not
a separate syndrome and that the behavioural features portrayed in the PDA
children can be found within individuals with a diagnosis of ASD, having said
this, they also consider PDA research to be ‘innovative’ and clinically useful.
“Individuals with PDA tend to have over-active imagination
as opposed to under-active, and this clearly sets them apart from Wing's
description of the autistic Triad of Impairments.” (PDA Contact Centre, 2012).
The
above statement reifies the idea that autistic people lack imagination, and
that someone displaying imagination in some external sense could not possibly
be autistic and thus PDA would be a more accurate descriptor. This is a misinterpretation in my view of the
autistic mind-set however, where the apparent rigidity of many on the spectrum
can be due to a number of factors, from monotropic focus (Murray et al. 2005)
to stress and overload (Milton, 2012c) or a need to control one’s external
environment (which is exactly the same reasons purported to be creating an
avoidance of demands in descriptions of PDA).
A major difficulty in suggesting that a behavioural manifestation is not
autistic, is that to make such a statement one would have to have a good idea
of what autism is, and this should be anything but a presumed given,
considering the lack of clarity and explanatory value the dominant
psychological theories contain (Milton, 2011).
“It is important to remember that PDA is not caused by a
person's upbringing or their social circumstances and it is not the fault of
the parents or the individual with the condition.” (Christie and Duncan, 2012).
There
is simply not enough evidence to support a claim such as this, especially when
there are similar traits associated with both developmental and attachment
disorders. It is more than possible that
autistic people can be traumatised by social relationships and by negating such
factors as ever having a causative association with avoidance behaviours could
be potentially negligent. Unfortunately
current theories regarding attachment and trauma are not much more coherent
than that regarding PDA.
Educational discourse regarding PDA children
“...many parents describe their child as working harder to
avoid the demand than she would have done by accepting it. Whatever the child’s
intellectual level…educational support will need to be geared to helping the
child to tolerate ‘being educated’ to the greatest degree possible, in order at
least to approach her potential.” (PDA
Contact Centre, 2012).
In
this statement, the PDA Contact Centre (2012) equates ‘being educated’ with
conformity to non-autistic hegemonic practices.
The notion that a child on the autism spectrum would have to work less
to comply with demands than to reject them displays a total lack of empathy
with many an autistic perspective. As a
case in point, I was once asked to summarise autism in three words, the answer
I gave was: “Natures answer to over-conformity…”.
According
to the PDA Contact Centre (2012) there are three main educational needs that
practitioners working with such children need to consider:
-
Keeping the child on task for
a substantial period of the day.
-
Ensuring that what she appears
to be learning is actually absorbed and retained.
-
Ensuring that a minimal degree
of disruption to other children takes place, and trying to create positive peer
relationships despite the resentment such disruption can cause in other
children. Sometimes this will include the need to keep other children
physically safe.
Such
an educational agenda applied to any child, I would consider woefully
teacher-led, as it does not highlight at any point the building of a
relationship or an attempt to understand the educational context from the
position of the learner. The learner’s
role in this context is simply to stay on the task as envisaged by the non-autistic
adult.
“However, a mainstream school is appropriate wherever
possible, as PDA children are socially imitative and therefore good normative
models are important.” (PDA Contact, 2012).
The
above quote belies a damaging ideology that has been embedded within
behaviourist models of autism since the writings of Lovaas (1987) in that the
company of other autistic or neuro-divergent individuals would be a negative
influence on one another by modelling inappropriate behaviours. In the mind of this autistic person however,
nothing has been more disabling than being isolated from those with similar
dispositions (Milton, 2012b).
Christie (2007) suggests that diagnosis of
PDA should help to better understand an individual and to use that
understanding to help ‘formulate more effective forms of intervention and
provision’. As with most literature
regarding autism, the emphasis is thus on intervening with the perceived
deviant disorder in a remedial effort to normalise behaviour.
“...many of the generally
accepted strategies that are advocated for working for children with autism and
Asperger’s syndrome were not proving successful for children with PDA; an
altogether different emphasis was required...The use of structure, routine and
behavioural principles of reward that are usually effective for children with
autism or Asperger’s syndrome are rarely so for children with PDA.” (Christie,
2007).
Although somewhat redeeming this account by
reminding readers that no one set of guidelines is applicable to all on the
autism spectrum, Christie (2007) falls foul of assuming that behaviourist
principles as applied to autistic people are effective in the first place,
ignoring the discourse of autistic writers which are often quite to the contrary
(Dawson, 2004, Milton 2012b). Despite
recommending the building up of trust and mutual relationships with key workers
and the ‘Circles of Friends’ technique, the advice offered by Christie (2007)
holds conformity to appropriate normative behaviour as the ultimate outcome to
work toward, conformity and adaptation to the demands of the non-autistic world
rather than a truly mutual exchange.
“People with PDA tend to respond much better to a more
indirect and negotiative style that allows them to feel in control.” (Christie
and Duncan, 2012).
Such
an approach would also be recommended with autistic people more generally
(Milton, 2012c), yet the manner of such negotiation would be dependent on each
person and context, although sometimes it is beneficial to actually give
children (autistic or not) the power and control to make their own autonomous
decisions from time to time. This begs
the question: exactly who has a ‘pathological’ need to control whom?
The
construction of PDA can thus be seen as an emblem of contemporary biopower in
action (Foucault, 1973, Finkelstein, 1997), based upon the construction of self
in terms of the discourses of cognitive and developmental psychology reproduced
in educational ideology and practice, with those so labelled subjected to a
pathologising gaze and modified to meet idealised standards. Such a construction can be seen in its
contemporary social nexus as a reaction to individuals seen as a risk to
productivity and conformity to normative standards, in need of ‘discipline and
control’, a not-so ‘docile body’ to be transformed and modified so it becomes
as such.
Conclusion
In the eyes
of many psychologist, educational practitioner, and parent, there are many
people who would fit the criteria for PDA, however that does not mean the
conceptualisation of what is causing such behaviours is at all well understood. The PDA narrative presents what
is perfectly rational behaviour from the viewpoint of the autistic person
displaying it when faced with highly stressful situations, as not as a
consequence of ‘choice’ (whatever that may be) but as a pathological
response. It is deemed pathological due
the distaste of those doing the perceiving and their idealisation of cultural
and psychological norms.
The PDA
narrative suggests that those who gain some social interaction skills and
assert their needs through avoidance of imposed demands are pathological. In
essence, such protests are perceived as the fault of pathology inherent in the
individual mind rather than a conflicting interaction (much like 'theory of
mind'). It will no doubt lead to treatments that try and stop such behaviours
(which could be read as a form of self-advocacy and the gaining of skills). Such behaviours arise from any number of transactions between the
individual and environment. The
avoidance of demands is interactional in nature, and much like a lack of social
reciprocity cannot be located solely in the mind of any one individual (Milton,
2012a).
The label
of PDA represents the medicalising and pathologising of behaviours that from an
outsider perspective seem to be differentiated from what is deemed capable by
autistic people, but could be seen as the behaviours of an autistic person who
has gained a modicum of normative social skills and is simply asserting their
agency. By pathogising such behaviour,
one could unduly be blunting attempts at autistic self-advocacy. Part of this misperception is due to the
application of a deficit model of autism that considers autistic people as
incapable of displaying social agency. As
with other labels in the psychologisation of human social life, it is a
descriptive construction from a medicalised pathologising functionalist
discourse, yet even less than other labels it does not signpost practitioners
to the needs of the person, but to the needs of those around them. One could even argue that there is a struggle
for power embedded in the discourse, in which one could question who it is who
needs to control whom, an incidence of biopower in action.
Distortion
As the final door begins to close, we make do with what our
leaders impose.
What are they implying, what is the message that is underlying?
We are polluted by this infestation, I need an outlet for my
frustration.
I don’t want to comply, and I have no need to justify.
(Milton, 1989).
References
American Psychiatric Association (1994)
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th Edition) (DSM-IV),
Washington DC: APA.
Arnold,
L. (2010) The Medium is the Message, accessed
from: www.ucl.ac.uk/cpjh/Arnold, 08/10/12.
Christie,
P. (2007) The Distinctive Clinical and
Educational Needs of Children with Pathological Demand Avoidance Syndrome:
Guidelines for Good Practice (originally published in Good Autism Practice), Accessed from: http://www.aettraininghubs.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2012/05/5.2-strategies-for-teaching-pupils-with-PDA.pdf,
05/10/12.
Chirstie, P. and
Duncan, M. (2012) Pathological Demand
Avoidance. Accessed from:
www.autism.org.uk/15355, 05/10/12.
Dawson, M. (2004) The
misbehaviour of behaviourists: ethical challenges to the autism-ABA industry,
accessed at: http://www.sentex.net/~nexus23/naa_aba.html, on 12/02/11.
Finkelstein, V. (1997) Chic outrage and body politics. In K. Davis (ed.), Embodied Practices: Feminist Perspectives on the Body, pp.
150-169. London: Sage.
Foucault, M. (1973) Madness
and Civilisation: A History of Insanity in the Age of Reason (trans. R.
Howard). New York: Vintage.
Lovaas, O. (1987) ‘Behavioural treatment and normal educational
and intellectual functioning in young autistic children’, Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, Vol. 55, pp. 3-9.
Milton, D. (1989) Chapters of the Lifehouse [collected poems]. Unpublished.
Milton, D. (1999) The Rise of Psychopharmacology. Masters Degree Essay: University of
London.
Milton, D. (2011a) 'Who am I meant to be': in search of a
psychological model of autism from the viewpoint of an 'insider'. Critical Autism Seminar 18/01/11. Sheffield
Hallam University.
Milton, D. (2012a)
‘On the ontological status of autism: the double empathy problem’. Disability
and Society. Accessed from:
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09687599.2012.710008, 08/10/12.
Milton, D. (2012b) ‘The
Normalisation Agenda and the Psycho-Emotional Disablement of Autistic
People’. Critical Disability Studies Conference 13/09/12. Lancaster University.
Milton, D. (2012c) So what exactly is autism? Autism Education Trust. Accessed from: http://www.aettraininghubs.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2012/08/1_So-what-exactly-is-autism.pdf,
08/10/12.
Murray, D., Lesser, M. and Lawson, W. (2005)
‘Attention, monotropism and the diagnostic criteria for autism.’ Autism. Vol.
9(2), pp. 136-156.
Newson, E., Le Marechal, K. and David, C.
(2003) Pathological Demand Avoidance syndrome: a necessary distinction within
the pervasive developmental disorders, Archives
of Disease in Childhood, Vol. 88: 595-600.
PDA
Contact Centre (2012), accessed from: http://www.pdacontact.org.uk/frames/index.html,
05/10/12.
Simone,
R. (2010) Aspergirls: Empowering Females
with Asperger Syndrome. London:
Jessica Kingsley.
Sinclair, J. (1993) Don’t Mourn For Us. Accessed from: http://www.autreat.com/dont_mourn.html,
10/11/11.
Wing, L and Gould, J. (2002) Pathological
Demand Avoidance, London: National Autistic Society.
World Health Organisation (1992) The tenth
revision of the international classification of diseases and related health
problems (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.